Killer culture - divergent killer-whale cultures
Glen Martin
It's dinnertime for the killer whales. Unhurriedly, the 28 members of
the pod skirt the edges of a school of coho salmon, gradually forcing
the fish into a closely packed mass and assuring the whales an easy
catch. As they work, the whales "talk" volubly among themselves,
seemingly exchanging information. * Twenty miles to the south, the
dinner bell has also rung for another, much smaller group of killer
whales. Their hunt is different from that of the salmon eaters. These
orcas are quietly approaching a basaltic shelf used as a resting place
by Steller's sea lions. The seal hunters disdain fish; they kill and
eat only marine mammals. They travel alone or in twos or threes rather
than in the large, freewheeling aggregations that characterize their
salmon-eating kin. And they are usually laconic. They never speak at
all while hunting. What conversation they do have is reserved for the
moments during and just after the kill, when the spoils of meat are
divided among the group.
The differences between these two groups have led researchers to
believe that the life of the killer whale - Orcinus orca - is a complex
one, based on traditions and cetacean-style family values. Apparently
these whales exist within the context of a genuine culture - or more
appropriately, cultures. "Orca societies are in no way similar to human
societies, but they are societies nevertheless," says whale biologist
Ken Balcomb.
This view has taken shape gradually over the past 20 years as
researchers have gathered more and more information on the killer whale
populations of the Pacific Northwest. Their data are of essentially two
types: photo identification of individual whales, and underwater
recordings of the sounds they make. Orcas may look alike to the
layperson-black with a white belly patch extending up the flanks, a
white patch behind the eye and one behind the dorsal fin, and a body up
to 30 feet long that weighs in the neighborhood of five to six tons -
but individual differences are obvious to the veteran researcher. "The
shape and condition of the dorsal fin and the size and shape of skin
patterns vary from whale to whale," says Balcomb. "When you examine
photos, each whale looks very distinctive."
Just as photos have allowed Balcomb and his fellow researchers to
follow the lives of individual whales, hydrophones have allowed them to
document the linguistic and aural lives of the various groups. Together
these two approaches have helped lead researchers to the conclusion
that two separate populations of killer whales inhabit the waters off
British Columbia and southeastern Alaska, populations so different that
they are essentially like different species.
Field observers have dubbed the two groups "residents" and
"transients," rubrics that emphasize their most distinguishing
characteristics. Residents, the homeboys of the orca world, demarcate
well-defined ranges, in which they follow and herd five separate
species of salmon. Transients, by contrast, are free-ranging nomads;
they cruise far and wide along the western coast of North America,
taking harbor seals, sea lions, porpoises, and other whales where they
find them.
Balcomb's specialty is the residents, and he lives in an ideal
venue for their study. He works at the Center for Whale Research, which
is located on San Juan Island, just off the coast of Washington State.
Perched on the rocky shore of Haro Strait, in between San Juan and
Vancouver islands, the site offers a clear view of hundreds of square
miles of protected inland waterway. Hydrophones sunk offshore allow
Balcomb to hear the whales coming; their squeals and squawks can be
picked up while they are several miles away. During the summer, when
the chinook and coho salmon are running, three separate pods of whales
(a total of 96 animals) breeze by with almost metronomic regularity. In
all, 19 different resident pods inhabit the near-shore waters that run
from the San Juan Islands to the tip of southeastern Alaska some 600
miles away. "It's taken us years," says Balcomb, "but we've identified
every separate pod of resident orcas, and every individual within a
pod, here in the San Juans and in the Strait of Georgia, which is just
to the north. And we've learned a tremendous amount about their social
interactions."
A salient characteristic of a resident society is its matriarchal
structure. Orcas are shy breeders. They reproduce slowly, averaging one
calf every eight years (a total of fewer than 2,000 orcas are thought
to inhabit Alaskan and western Canadian waters). But the resident
calves that are produced stick around. "Sons and daughters stay with
the mothers through their entire life span of 40 to 80 years," says
Balcomb. "That's very rare in the wild." And when the pod grows to an
unwieldy size, it splits matrilineally. "You end up with groups of
whales that live as extended families, with females at the top of the
hierarchy."
Residents also distinguish themselves by sticking close to areas
where food is known to be plentiful; salmon are reliably found in
certain parts of the ocean at certain times of the year, and whales
that consistently hunt those parts assure themselves of food.
Most significantly, though, resident orcas are loquacious. Their
vocabularies are among the richest in the cetacean kingdom. "Contrary
to popular belief, not all whales vocalize extensively," says marine
mammal scientist John Ford of the Vancouver Aquarium. "For example,
most baleen whales, with the exception of humpbacks, are fairly quiet."
Of the cetaceans that have been studied, well-defined vocalizations
seem to be the exclusive province of toothed whales like the orcas,
along with dolphins and humpbacks. Not surprisingly, perhaps, the
talkers seem to be more intelligent than the nontalkers.
"Many baleen whale species, which vocalize very little, don't
appear to be that bright," says Marilyn Dahlheim, a researcher with the
National Marine Mammal Laboratory in Seattle. "Gray whales, for one,
seem to be about as smart as cows." Of course, she says, it's hard to
equate vocalization with intelligence, and who's to say cows are not
intelligent? After all, it's not that the krill-eating baleen whales
are particularly stupid; it's just that they are only as bright as they
need to be. "Most baleen whales feed on species they can forage for,
and they don't need a great deal of acumen to track them down," says
Ford. "But you have to be pretty smart to hunt a seal or a porpoise."
By this reckoning, the relatively high IQ of the humpbacks makes
sense. Unlike other baleen whales, humpbacks feed heavily on small fish
such as herring and sardines. They capture them with their own version
of a fisherman's net. Several humpbacks will join forces to compress a
school of fish by blowing streams of bubbles around the school's
periphery. When the fish are packed tight, the whales dive into the
middle of them with jaws agape. The cooperative action necessary for
such foraging behavior presumably requires more intelligence than the
vagrant straining of the seas for krill as practiced by the gray, blue,
and other baleen whales.
Orcas and humpbacks, then, are among a handful of species that have
been "unleashed from the restraints of genetics," as Ford puts it. They
have developed the specialized organs and brain structures that make
vocal learning both possible and useful. Researchers disagree over
whether the sounds and songs produced by orcas and other whales
constitute an actual language. But Ford believes they do, at least for
orcas, although he cautions that orca vocalizations don't begin to
approximate human speech in complexity.
"Only humans, macaque monkeys, and some whales and seals can learn,
reproduce, and even modify sounds," he says; some researchers, he
notes, would include certain bird species as well. Yet orcas not only
have language - they have dialects. Ford has determined that each
resident pod speaks a lingo that's distinct even from the language of
other pods living nearby. Dialects are evidently determined by family
roots rather than geography.
Ford has identified several calls that have specific meanings for
each of the resident pods off the San Juans and British Columbia. "We
divided the pods into different clans based on our identification of
their dialects," he says. "We believe that some pods have close
consanguinity with other pods and share some of the same calls. But
pods that we think are more distantly related use different calls in
any given circumstance."
The concept, he says, is analogous to human language. "One clan,
for example, may use orca Romance languages - different pods speak
Spanish, Italian, or French within the larger clan context. But another
clan is, say, Chinese, and different pods within the clan may speak
Cantonese, Mandarin, or some other dialect."
By recording and comparing different calls, Ford has found that
within a single pod, each call will have a specific acoustic pattern -
one call may consist of three warbles with an overall rise in pitch,
then an abrupt drop-off at the end. In a related pod, that same call
might maintain the three warbles and the rise in pitch but be missing
the drop-off. Ford also suspects that the two characteristics unique to
resident orcas - possession of dialects and the matriarchal structure
of the pods - could be linked. Because sons and daughters remain with
the mother's pod their entire lives, the danger of inbreeding should
exist. Different dialects may allow the whales to identify different
lineages for the purpose of mating.
Residents within any given pod average 12 different calls, all of
which may vary in duration and pitch depending on the whales' activity.
When they are associating with whales from other pods, for example, the
animals become excited. Then their calls become more frequent and
varied, and they tend to be pitched higher. "You can tell their mood
right away," says Ford. "Their emotional state seems to be encoded in
sound more than anything else."
Residents also freely use echolocation to find their prey; these
sonar clicks are clearly distinct from the lilting trills and squeaks
that constitute their social interactions. "No one knows if any of the
orcas form an actual image in their brains from their echolocation
signals," says Robin Baird, an orca researcher from Simon Fraser
University in Vancouver. "We don't know as much about orca echolocation
as we do for the smaller dolphins. But certain dolphins can
discriminate between objects of different sizes and compositions to a
very fine degree. Since orcas echolocate for the same purpose as
dolphins - to find food - it's likely they share similar abilities."
But while all orcas echolocate, not all do it to the same degree.
"Residents often echolocate in a long series of clicks," Baird says.
"They're probably getting very detailed pictures of the salmon
schools." It's a different story with transient orcas. They, too,
possess the ability to echolocate, but they rarely use it. "And when
they do," says Baird, "it's usually in single clicks." The difference
between resident and transient echolocation behavior may stem from the
different diets of the two groups. Marine mammals - the prey of choice
for the transients - may become alarmed by the sound of incoming
echolocation, so the transients may have learned that a silent strategy
works best for hunting.
Certainly some marine mammals seem able to tell which orcas are a
threat. "Harbor seals are the main prey for transients around the
southern island," says Baird. "Many of the seals show only mild
interest when residents swim near them, but they become noticeably
alarmed when transients come into the vicinity."
While transients may have learned to be quiet during a hunt, they
do talk extravagantly once a kill is under way. "They vocalized
tremendously during a gray whale kill we witnessed," says Sean Van
Sommeran, the operations director of the Pelagic Shark Research
Foundation in Santa Cruz, California. "The orcas killed the gray by
drowning and buffeting it. They hit it with their flukes, and they bit
the pectorals and pulled it underwater so it couldn't get air." After
the kill, Van Sommeran witnessed mothers escorting their calves to the
carcass to deliver a few fluke blows. "Sometimes the young ones would
miscalculate the blows, flip completely out of the water, and land on
the carcass. I think it's the kind of technique that takes considerable
practice to perform correctly." And through it all, says Van Sommeran,
the whales were extremely noisy.
Still, such calls may be akin to simple war whoops or cheers;
transient vocabularies are truncated compared with those of the
residents. It appears they use no more than half a dozen distinct
calls, a kind of pared-down "hunt language." Their language is uniform
too, with only a single dialect spoken from Alaska's Glacier Bay as far
south as Los Angeles. "The transients probably don't need as many calls
as the residents to define their social interactions," says Ford. "They
travel in smaller groups, the young don't always stay with their
mothers, and they don't stay and hunt in a particular area. There just
isn't as much long-term stability in transient groups, so their
languages haven't had a chance to evolve."
Research on transients is not as advanced as it is on residents,
chiefly because transients move around a lot and are hard to find. Yet
Van Sommeran believes it's logical to assume they repeatedly visit
certain hunting locations where they've had success. "We think they
show up at specific places at specific times of the year when they know
marine mammals will be abundant," he says. "One month they'll be in
Monterey when the gray females are crossing the bay with their calves,
another time they'll head to a rookery when seals or sea lions are
having their pups." Other researchers aren't so sure that transients
shadow migrating pinnipeds and whales the way lions shadow migrating
wildebeests in Kenya, but all acknowledge that their range is
impressive. Dahlheim has shown that the North Pacific transients range
from Alaska to at least central California and probably farther south.
Transients and residents are distinct biologically as well as
culturally, though no one is yet ready to declare that the two groups
constitute different species or subspecies. "We've documented small
morphological differences very well," says Balcomb. "Residents have
definitely evolved rounded dorsal fins, while transient dorsals are
quite pointed. And the white dorsal saddle patch goes forward much
farther on the transients than on the residents."
Recent mitochondrial DNA tests conducted on captive transient and
resident killer whales from the Vancouver Island area indicate that
genetically, the transients are as different from the local resident
whales as they are from orcas in the North Atlantic. The rather loose
structure of transient society apparently allows ample opportunity for
gene swapping between the various transient groups. But the tests
conducted so far suggest that no Vancouver Island orca females have
broken "social" lines to crossbreed for a very long time - for many
thousands of years at least. Female residents breed with male
residents, and female transients with male transients.
Which group came first? Researchers speculate that the residents
were originally "seeded" from the transients: Free to range the world's
oceans, the primordial ancestors of today's orcas sought seals and
whales from pole to pole. In those areas where fish were plentiful and
available year-round, it's likely some orcas stayed to take advantage
of the bounty. And because catching fish required different social and
foraging skills from those required for catching seals, new habits and
new cultures evolved.
Separation between resident and transient populations occurs in
parts of the globe other than the Pacific Northwest. Orcas are the
world's most widely distributed marine mammals, though they are not
particularly numerous in any locale they inhabit - about what you'd
expect for an animal that lives at the very pinnacle of the marine food
chain. "In Antarctica the situation is very similar to the one we have
here," says Balcomb. "There some of the orcas subsist on a type of
small, oily cod that exists in huge numbers, while other groups live
exclusively on whales and pinnipeds. And each avoids the other."
Residents and transients seem to have one final behavioral
difference that researchers can't help noticing: the two have radically
different attitudes toward intrusive humans. Residents tolerate - even
welcome - human propinquity. The photographs of kayakers lolling among
orca dorsal fins commonly featured in Alaskan and British Columbian
travel brochures portray resident whales. No similar photographs exist
of transients, apparently because they are, at best, aloof. There have
been no substantiated transient attacks on humans, but posturings that
can easily be translated as aggressive are not uncommon.
"During one gray whale kill, a big male transient swam out to our
boat, splashed us with water, then backed away," says Van Sommeran. "It
clearly seemed as if he wanted us to keep our distance. So we did."
Dahlheim also remembers a chancy encounter with a transient female off
Baja after a sea lion kill. "She was with her calf and had a big chunk
of blubber in her mouth," says Dahlheim. "She came porpoising right up
to our boat and began slapping her flukes against the water. I'd
hesitate to say she meant us harm, because I doubt I'd be here if she
was determined to hurt us. But I did feel intimidated." Ford puts it
more succinctly: "The more I learn about transients, the less I'm
inclined to go skinny-dipping with them."
Much has been learned about the behavior of resident and transient
orcas, but the researchers acknowledge that their understanding is just
beginning. Indeed, some scientists think the notion of "transient" and
"resident" populations may be outmoded simply because there could be
more than two distinct orca societies out there in the deep waters of
the Pacific Northwest.
"About a year ago we discovered a group of orcas that weren't
residents or transients - we'd never seen them before," says Balcomb.
"Their language was different from any we've recorded, and they were
farther offshore than orcas normally are - so we're calling them the
|offshore' orcas. We know absolutely nothing about them; they're a
complete mystery."
Because their habits and environment make them difficult to study,
orcas will most likely provide us with plenty of mystery for a long
time to come. But rudimentary as it is, the information that has been
collected is enough to dispel some egregious myths. Until the middle
decades of this century, orcas were viewed as rapacious murderers of
the deep, wont to slay gentle baleen whales and doe-eyed seals - even
unfortunate humans - in obeisance to an unslaked blood lust. Later they
were transformed into antic "sea pandas," happy-go-lucky cetaceans
whose highest purpose in life was to turn somersaults in a pool or snap
a mackerel from a trainer's hand for the delight of tourists. Neither
portrait, however, is accurate.
"These are extremely intelligent animals, and they live in very
sophisticated social groups," says Balcomb, who's recently been
spearheading an effort to establish radio linkups with captive resident
killer whales and their home pods, to see how they will react if
allowed to communicate directly with one another. "We're still learning
about them, but we do know they're nothing like the caricatures we've
assumed them to be."
COPYRIGHT 1993 Discover
COPYRIGHT 2004 Gale Group